瑪麗安娜•沃爾夫(Maryanne Wolf)
★美國塔夫茨大學兒童發展心理學教授,閱讀與語言研究中心主任。
★曾獲富布萊特奬,並因齣色的教學和科研工作,獲得美國心理學會、美國國傢兒童健康與人類發展研究所以及國際閱讀障礙者協會頒發的奬項。
★《普魯斯特與烏賊》獲得瑪格•梅爾剋年度最佳閱讀圖書奬。
★《華盛頓郵報》稱贊她“說的任何事都具有一定的意義,她還真正預言瞭計算機文化對‘閱讀思維’的影響”。
Anyone who reads is bound to wonder, at least occasionally, about how those funny squiggles on a page magically turn into "Bare ruined choirs, where late the sweet birds sang" or "After a while I went out and left the hospital and walked back to the hotel in the rain." Where did this unlikely skill called reading come from? What happens in our brain when our eyes scan a line of type? Why do some of us, or some of our children, find it difficult to process the visual information held in words?
In Proust and the Squid, Maryanne Wolf, a professor at Tufts University and director of its Center for Reading and Language Research, offers explanations for all these questions, but with an emphasis that is "more biological and cognitive than cultural-historical." This means that Wolf focuses on the physiological character of the human brain, which holds at its disposal "three ingenious design principles: the capacity to make new connections among older structures; the capacity to form areas of exquisitely precise specialization for recognizing patterns in information, and the ability to learn to recruit and connect information from these areas automatically." These "design principles" provide the neuronal foundation of reading, and Wolf spends half her book explaining the evolution and minutiae of this "reading brain."
Nearly all this material makes for very hard slogging, even though Proust and the Squid is confidently described as the author's "first book for the general public." (The catchy but utterly uninformative title, by the way, refers to the novelist's impressionistic thoughts about childhood reading and a scientist's use of the squid brain for neurological research.) A work of popularization needs a light clear style, lots of anecdotes and some plot or story line that moves along at a good clip. At times, Wolf makes a stab at including some human-interest element or personal example, but all too soon she reverts to her normal prose, which is austere, technical and, finally, wearisome:
"In a pathbreaking meta-analysis of twenty-five imaging studies of different languages, cognitive scientists from the University of Pittsburgh found three great common regions used differentially across writing systems. In the first, the occipital-temporal area (which includes the hypothesized locus of 'neuronal recycling' for literacy), we become proficient visual specialists in whatever script we read. In the second, the frontal region around Broca's area, we become specialists in two different ways -- for phonemes in words and for their meanings. In the third, the multifunction region spanning the upper temporal lobes and the lower, adjacent parietal lobes, we recruit additional areas that help to process multiple elements of sounds and meanings, which are particularly important for alphabetic and syllabary systems."
Out of context such prose sounds perfectly dreadful -- and in context sadly characteristic of the writing in professional journals, no matter what the field. In fact, everything Wolf says makes sense, the specialized terms she uses have been previously defined, and there are line illustrations on a facing page. Nonetheless, such technical onslaughts are extremely tiring to read, and Wolf seldom lets up on the information-rich barrage for very long. At different points she does quote passages from Proust and George Eliot, but even these two great novelists are hardly what you'd call sprightly, and they merely add their own specific gravity to already forbidding pages.
In the second half of the book, Wolf examines the reading difficulties generally subsumed under the term dyslexia. We learn that one of her sons suffers from this disability, that there are various forms and theories about its origin and character, that it can sometimes result in a special talent for fields that emphasize pattern and spatial creativity (such as art, design and engineering) and that "programs which systematically and explicitly teach young readers phoneme awareness and grapheme-phoneme correspondence are far more successful in dealing with reading disabilities than other programs." As this last sentence makes evident, no relief awaits the once-eager reader who by this point has begun to wonder if he could be suffering from a sudden case of adult-onset dyslexia.
Despite Wolf's failure to write a truly popular book, she clearly does know her stuff, and those professionally involved with the teaching of reading might be more patient than I. In particular, she addresses the special needs of children raised in cultures where standard English isn't the dominant language, and she speculates, with real concern, about the impact of computer culture on the "reading brain." Dyslexia has taught her that humans were never genetically designed to read, and this peculiar technique of sustained mental attention could be reduced, reconfigured or even lost in the rising digital age:
"Will unguided information lead to an illusion of knowledge, and thus curtail the more difficult, time-consuming, critical thought processes that lead to knowledge itself? Will the split-second immediacy of information gained from a search engine and the sheer volume of what is available derail the slower, more deliberative processes that deepen our understanding of complex concepts, of another's inner thought processes, and of our own consciousness?"
Wolf never fully answers these questions, though they strike me as the basis for a much needed book. Still, like any parent with a child transfixed by flashing screens, she is troubled by what she observes. She urges that we "teach our children to be 'bitextual' " or 'multitextual,' able to read and analyze texts flexibly in different ways" so that our sons and daughters don't end up as mere "decoders of information," distracted from the "deeper development of their intellectual potential." Early on in Proust and the Squid, she had noted that infants and toddlers who aren't told stories by their caregivers, who aren't read to from a very early age, nearly always fail to learn to read well themselves. By implication, it may already be too late for many young people: They will never be able to read with the same thoughtfulness and comprehension as their parents. Think about that.
發表於2024-11-26
Proust and the Squid 2024 pdf epub mobi 電子書 下載
作者用瞭大量的解剖學發現和神經係統造影結果去說明個人從嬰兒時期是如何跨越一個個階段學會閱讀,人類不同語言的書寫係統是如何形成並影響人類閱讀腦的發展,以及閱讀障礙所帶來的對閱讀腦機製的新啓示。讀完這本書確實帶來瞭不少啓發。 作者認為,閱讀是一種人類迴收舊有神...
評分聲稱要對閱讀進行科學研究,就像聲稱對愛情進行科學研究一樣,會被絕大多數人看成是對人性的貶低,是對人世間的“魔法”冥頑不靈的否認。好在我們這個時代的科技工作者尚缺乏探索人類復雜感受與認知活動的能力。目前,這些頑固的實證主義者還隻能藉助腦成像對我們的閱讀體驗探...
評分聲稱要對閱讀進行科學研究,就像聲稱對愛情進行科學研究一樣,會被絕大多數人看成是對人性的貶低,是對人世間的“魔法”冥頑不靈的否認。好在我們這個時代的科技工作者尚缺乏探索人類復雜感受與認知活動的能力。目前,這些頑固的實證主義者還隻能藉助腦成像對我們的閱讀體驗探...
評分1、推薦所有關心兒童、兒童閱讀、兒童學習的人閱讀本書,因為這本書除瞭可以讓我們尊重閱讀這件事之外,還可以讓我們尊重兒童。對於各方麵趨於固化的成人來說,如果不瞭解閱讀,不瞭解兒童,那麼很有可能會妨礙到兒童的良性發展。 2、這本書的可讀性其實很強,作者是基於科研成...
圖書標籤: 神經科學 Psychology 心理學 Reading 美國 科技 心理學 互聯網
前三章有大量關於書總體結構的重復信息,但是沒有足夠的例子展開說明。關於古代書寫係統的發展,書中也有一些小的錯誤。做為相關研究的從業者,本書的深度令人失望;對沒有任何相關背景的讀者來說這也恐怕不會是一本非常簡明的入門書。
評分:無
評分前三章有大量關於書總體結構的重復信息,但是沒有足夠的例子展開說明。關於古代書寫係統的發展,書中也有一些小的錯誤。做為相關研究的從業者,本書的深度令人失望;對沒有任何相關背景的讀者來說這也恐怕不會是一本非常簡明的入門書。
評分:無
評分:無
Proust and the Squid 2024 pdf epub mobi 電子書 下載